You are viewing a complimentary preview of this book. For options to unlock the full book, please login or visit our catalog to create a FlatWorld Account and see purchase options.
The Science of Psychology
Connections and Contemporary Issues

v1.0 Martin S. Shapiro

1.1 History and Approaches

Learning Objectives

  1. Discuss the varying historical views of the mind and brain, beginning with the ancient Greeks through the Age of Enlightenment. 

  2. Differentiate between dualism and monism.

  3. Compare and contrast structuralism and functionalism.

  4. Describe key features of Sigmund Freud’s psychoanalytic approach to psychology and identify which of his contributions are still relevant today.

  5. Discuss the approach of behaviorism.

  6. Explain what motivated the cognitive revolution and how this approach differs from behaviorism.

  7. Explain how humanistic psychology and positive psychology are similar.

  8. Describe a sociocultural approach to psychology. 

  9. Provide examples of how someone taking an evolutionary or neuroscience approach to psychology might seek answers to questions about behavior.

Psychology is a vast and diverse field that overlaps with many other disciplines, like biology, neuroscience, chemistry, anthropology, genetics, sociology, child development, and counseling. However, psychology began with philosophers who strived to understand the workings of the mind. This section briefly looks at ideas about the mind and brain, from ancient Greece to the Age of Enlightenment. We’ll then look at the transitions in the late 1800s from philosophy to the emergence of the new field of psychology as a natural science. The section ends by examining some of the different approaches to the study of psychology over the past century and a half and some of the most significant psychologists and research within each approach. 

Ancient Greece, the Renaissance, and the Age of Enlightenment

Ancient Greek philosophers such as Aristotle and Socrates wrote about the nature of consciousness and what influenced memories, perceptions, and motivation (Hunt, 2009). However, like the ancient Egyptians, early Greek philosophers and physicians thought the heart and other organs were where our thoughts and intelligence resided. This misplaced anatomy was not true for all Greek philosophers, however: Plato, Pythagoras, and Hippocrates began to recognize the brain’s function in thought and reasoning (Gross, 1987). As early as 6500 BC, psychological problems were also thought to be located in the head, as a common treatment was drilling holes in the head in a process called trepanation to release evil spirits. The practice of drilling holes and digging around in the brain as a remedy for psychological disorders continued well into the 20th century with frontal lobotomies. 

During the Renaissance in the 14th through 17th centuries, we emerged from the dark ages and began to see artists like Leonardo da Vinci (1452–1519) and Michelangelo (1475–1564) depict brain anatomy in their work (refer to Figure 1.1). The Renaissance was a time when philosophers and writers like Francis Bacon (1561–1626) began to look at humans in relation to nature. Bacon wrote about ways of understanding the mind by observing behavior (MacDonald, 2007). The idea that the mind is a product of the natural world and that behavior reveals the workings of the mind is the cornerstone of many aspects of modern psychology. 

Figure 1.1 Brain in Art

Michelangelo’s painting The Creation of Adam (A) shows God and angels within what looks like the sagittal cut of a human brain. Compare this to the illustration in B.

On the left is Michelangelo’s painting labeled “A” and on the right is an image of half of the brain labeled “B”.

Long Description

The Creation of Adam shows Adam, mostly nude, reclining on the grassy Earth, with his left arm outstretched. God and the angels are on the right with the sky, or heaven, in the background. God’s arm is outstretched and his forefinger is touching Adam’s hand. The shape of image of God and the angels is very similar to the image of the brain on the right, showing the smooth center surrounded by the curvy lines of the brain mass.

After the Renaissance came the Age of Enlightenment and the scientific revolution during the 17th and 18th centuries, when great philosophers, thinkers, and mathematicians emerged, like René Descartes (1596–1650), John Locke (1632–1704), and David Hume (1711–1776). This was a time of —the idea that knowledge begins by observing the natural world. Empiricism is the beginning of the scientific method. René Descartes (1596–1650) introduced —the notion that the physical body and brain are separate from but influenced by the nonphysical mind or spirit (refer to Figure 1.2). A more modern approach to psychology is , which is the idea that the mind (consciousness) is a product of brain activity.

Figure 1.2 René Descartes’ Illustration of Mind/Body Dualism

Descartes believed that communication between thoughts and consciousness in our mind and the physical brain took place at the pineal gland, which is a small structure in the middle of the brain.

Black and white illustration of a person sitting, with their left hand pointed upward. From each eye are three lines, connecting to points A, B, and C on the right.

Long Description

The person’s body is sketched with lines, and their raised arm shows one solid arm with another one just below it, indicating movement. The solid finger has a dotted line going to point B and the finger below it to point C. In the person’s brain is a small teardrop shaped diagram with C, O, H, and G marked. Lines point from the diagram to lines behind each eye. The eyes are large circles, and within the ovals the lines connecting to points A, B, and C begin.

Approaches to Psychology

Psychology developed as its own field in the latter part of the 19th century. This study of complex behavior and the minds of humans and other animals has produced several approaches and subfields. 

Structuralism

By the mid-1800s, researchers in other sciences focused on the essential or most basic elements of their areas of study and built complexity from these components. For example, atoms make up molecules, cells make up tissue, and the laws of gravity help explain the movement of stars and planets. Understanding the complex human mind and the nature of consciousness by understanding the underlying components or structures was one of the first significant approaches to psychology. This way of thinking is called and was developed by German physiologist and philosopher Wilhelm Wundt (1832–1920). Many consider Wundt to be the father of experimental psychology. Founding the first psychology laboratory, he and his students created experimental methods to test human senses, perceptions, and memory both systematically and accurately (refer to Figure 1.3). The goal was to determine the fundamental units or building blocks of the human mind. Structuralism was the first attempt to apply the rigors of science to investigate mental processes.

Figure 1.3 Wilhelm Wundt’s Laboratory

Wilhelm Wundt is shown sitting at a table with instruments designed to test physical reactions and perceptions.

Wundt with gray hair and a long beard is seated, with five gentlemen in suits and ties observing. A chalkboard is behind them. 

Functionalism

was a school of thought about psychology and the human mind that focused on the function or purpose behind behaviors and thoughts. Functionalism was highly influenced by Charles Darwin’s (1809–1882) theory of natural selection to explain the evolution of organisms. According to Darwin’s theories, organisms have biological or behavioral adaptations that serve the function of helping them survive and reproduce. While structuralism was about the fundamental components of the mind and behavior, functionalism was about the motives and ultimate usefulness that consciousness brings (refer to “Two Early Approaches: Structuralism and Functionalism”). William James (1842–1910) was the founder of functionalism and is considered the father of American psychology. James wrote several books on psychology, including the 1,200-page The Principles of Psychology (1890), and he often ranks at the top of lists of the most influential psychologists of all time (Korn, Davis, & Davis, 1991). 

Two Early Approaches: Structuralism and Functionalism

This video compares and contrasts structuralism and functionalism as approaches to psychology. 

2.4 to 4.8- For a long time, questions
of human motivation
4.8 to 7.68and behavior were considered
a part of philosophy.
7.68 to 9.6Philosophers asked many of the questions
9.6 to 11.97that underlie psychological study today,
11.97 to 14.19particularly the debate
over whether behavior
14.19 to 17.64and personality are shaped
more by nature or nurture
17.64 to 19.71goes all the way back to the Greeks.
19.71 to 22.2But philosophers didn't go
about answering these questions
22.2 to 24.96in systematic or scientific ways.
24.96 to 27.21Their theories couldn't
be proved right or wrong
27.21 to 30.48because they were based
only on casual observation.
30.48 to 32.7Psychology really got
going as a discipline
32.7 to 34.89when two men decided
to take the principles
34.89 to 36.84of scientific research and apply them
36.84 to 38.85to the study of human behavior.
38.85 to 40.98Wilhelm Wundt was a German scientist
40.98 to 43.44who founded a laboratory in Leipzig
43.44 to 45.963that took a structuralist
approach to psychology.
45.963 to 48.96William James was an American
who founded a laboratory
48.96 to 51.664at Harvard that took a
functionalist approach.
51.664 to 54.739We'll take a closer look at
both men and their approaches
54.739 to 56.76in order to more thoroughly understand
56.76 to 58.983psychology's foundation as a science.
59.82 to 63.78Wundt's lab, founded in 1879,
was the first of its kind.
63.78 to 65.1His structuralist approach
65.1 to 67.02sought to identify the building blocks,
67.02 to 70.26or the structure, of
psychological experience.
70.26 to 73.26Other sciences had been broken
down in this way before.
73.26 to 75.87Chemistry had its periodic
table of elements,
75.87 to 78.36physics had its fundamental laws.
78.36 to 80.85Wundt sought to do the
same for psychology,
80.85 to 83.16establishing a series
of fundamental relations
83.16 to 86.163or structures that could be
used to explain all behavior.
87.03 to 90.18Wundt and colleagues, like
student Edward Titchener,
90.18 to 93.21used a method called introspection
to learn what was going
93.21 to 96.3through people's heads as
they completed various tasks.
96.3 to 97.68Wundt was especially interested
97.68 to 100.5in how people processed sensory stimuli
100.5 to 103.35and he was the first to draw
distinction between sensation,
103.35 to 106.17or a stimulus' effect
on one of our senses,
106.17 to 107.79and perception, or our brain's
107.79 to 110.07interpretation of the stimulus.
110.07 to 112.44He discovered this by realizing
that when he asked people
112.44 to 115.464to listen to a sound and respond
as soon as they heard it,
115.464 to 117.99they were much faster when
all they had to say was
117.99 to 120.15whether they heard it rather than
120.15 to 122.67if they had to say what sound they heard.
122.67 to 124.17The processing and categorizing
124.17 to 126.72of sound took longer
than the hearing of it,
126.72 to 128.73indicating that perception is a process
128.73 to 131.343that is separate but related to sensation.
132.24 to 134.28This is a fundamental
concept for psychology
134.28 to 135.75that Wundt was able to discover
135.75 to 137.809through subjects' introspection.
137.809 to 139.65The problem with introspection though,
139.65 to 141.54as you've maybe probably guessed,
141.54 to 143.58is that people's descriptions
of their own feelings
143.58 to 145.89and reactions are often wrong.
145.89 to 147.78Let's say a psychologist
like Wundt were trying
147.78 to 150.6to figure out the softest
sound a person could hear.
150.6 to 152.97He'd play a sound, and then
ask if you could hear it,
152.97 to 154.35relying on your introspection
154.35 to 156.96to determine how sharp your hearing is.
156.96 to 158.883But if you expected to hear a sound.

Psychoanalytic Approach

There is probably no more iconic association with psychology than Sigmund Freud (1856–1939) and his . Freud had many ideas on child development, personality, consciousness, therapy, repressed memories or sexual thoughts, and unconscious thoughts. In psychoanalysis therapy, people are encouraged to release repressed thoughts and desires from deep in their unconscious minds to help resolve conflicts. Individuals would lie on Freud’s couch and talk freely (free association) about dreams, desires, and childhood memories, from which Freud felt he could access their unconscious thoughts and desires.

Freud was also influenced by Darwin’s theories, emphasizing that humans and animals likely have similar basic wants, desires, and instincts. Freudian ideas and therapy techniques have fallen out of favor as they are not easily scientifically investigated for effectiveness (refer to “Why Do We Still Teach Freud If He Was So Wrong?”). Freudian theory, however, has influenced many psychologists and researchers over the past century. Freud was responsible for emphasizing the role of the unconscious mind, which plays a significant role in many aspects of psychology, including perception, motivation, learning, and memory. 

Several influential women also did pioneering work taking the psychoanalytic approach. Sigmund Freud’s daughter, Anna Freud (1885–1982), continued her father’s work and helped develop the field of child psychotherapy. Karen Horney (1885–1952) pushed back against many of Sigmund Freud’s emphases on repressed sexual drives and founded feminist psychology, ensuring that women were represented fairly in research and psychoanalytic theories.

Why Do We Still Teach Freud If He Was So Wrong?

This video is a discussion about Sigmund Freud’s legacy in the field of psychology, including what is relevant today.

Behaviorism

The ideas of functionalism influenced the field of . As the name implies, behaviorism is about understanding how humans and other animals change with experience by observing and recording their behavior. Behaviorists often use animal models like dogs, rats, and pigeons to conduct well-controlled research with the idea that rules that govern behavior in animals can be applied to people. Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov (1849–1936) applied what he had discovered about the digestive system and his knowledge of the nervous system to investigate the rules and mechanisms of learning (refer to “Classical Conditioning—Ivan Pavlov”). Through his research, Pavlov found that by ringing a bell and giving a dog food, the dog would eventually learn to associate the sound of the bell with food and would salivate when it heard the bell, a connection made unconsciously in the brain. About the same time that Pavlov was developing his theories of learning in Russia, American psychologist Edward Thorndike (1874–1849) developed theories of how reward and punishment influenced learning and modified animal behavior. Pavlov and Thorndike developed generalized laws of learning that applied to most animals. 

Classical Conditioning—Ivan Pavlov

This video is hosted by psychologist Philip Zimbardo discussing behaviorism and the work of Ivan Pavlov.

Other behaviorists, such as John Watson (1878–1958) and B. F. Skinner (1904–1990), focused more on observing and predicting behavior. According to Watson and Skinner, consciousness and mental functions are beyond our observation and violate empiricism: If we cannot see and record it, then it is out of bounds for good science. However, tangible rewards (food or praise) and punishments (shock or loud noises) shape observable behaviors in predictable ways. The rules discovered to shape the behavior in animals could be used in child development, language acquisition, training animals, and helping people with disabilities. Skinner called his type of behaviorism radical behaviorism, which focuses primarily on observing, recording, manipulating, and predicting behavior. B. F. Skinner and radical behaviorism are still very influential today. His work is used in the field of applied behavior analysis, which helps children with psychological disorders like autism (refer to Figure 1.4).

Figure 1.4 Applied Behavior Analysis

A person trained in ABA works with a child on the autism spectrum to help them learn to identify facial expressions.

A woman and child sit across from each other at a table in a room decorated with toys. The child points to a blue face with a smile on it; the woman also holds a red frowny face.

Cognitive Approach

In the early part of the 20th century, there was strong pushback against behaviorism and the idea that animal learning and behavior were a product of stimuli, responses, and reinforcement. Perhaps animals think, anticipate, expect, and solve problems in their mind. This was known as the . While both fields are interested in understanding and predicting the behavior of both human and nonhuman animals, behaviorism strives to understand how environmental experiences (stimuli, reinforcements) shape behavior, while cognitive psychology focuses on the inner workings of the mind. Both cognitive psychology and behaviorism play significant roles in learning, memory, decision making, and therapy.

Humanistic Approach

Humanism is a term used in philosophy and psychology. As a philosophical movement, humanism flourished during the Renaissance and emphasized the individual’s leading a self-fulfilling life while still having responsibility for the greater good of society and other people. is an approach developed in the mid-20th century as an alternative to behaviorism and psychoanalytic approaches (refer to “What Even Is "Self-Actualization"? Humanistic Theory”). In some ways, behaviorism took away the free will of the individual, and Freudian theories were too pessimistic, focusing on deep-seated and unresolved conflicts. But humanistic psychology has a more optimistic view of humans and assumes that we have greater control and agency over our behaviors and emotions. The humanistic approach focuses on the idea that we should always strive to grow and reach our full potential. Carl Rogers (1902–1987), Charlotte Bühler (1893–1974), and Abraham Maslow (1908–1970) were some of the most influential humanistic psychologists. Rogers helped develop the person-centered approach to therapy and counseling. Maslow talked about the principles of human needs in our desire to be the best version of ourselves, what he called achieving self-actualization. 

What Even Is "Self-Actualization"? Humanistic Theory

This video shows a discussion of the history and theories associated with humanistic psychology.

Sociocultural Approach

The examines how society, culture, families, customs, and ethnicity shape our thoughts, emotions, learning, behaviors, and development. This approach stresses interaction with other people and suggests social interaction affects how we learn and grow. Culture is a rather broad term but typically describes the shared values, customs, norms, beliefs, and acquired knowledge of a group. It may encompass food, religion, art, music, and laws. Because of the broad influences on the individual, the sociocultural approach overlaps with sociology and anthropology. This approach began with Russian psychologist Lev Vygotsky (1896–1934), who emphasized the family and culture as the driving forces for child development.

Evolutionary Approach

An looks at psychology in terms of how evolution may have shaped emotions, motivations, perceptions, personalities, attitudes, and behaviors. It begins with Darwin’s theory of natural selection and how nature selects adaptive physical as well as behavioral characteristics. The evolutionary approach is interrelated with the study of genetics and the heritability of certain traits. Our ancient ancestors faced many challenges, and those who carried the genes for behavioral traits that were adaptive and helpful tended to, on average, survive better, reproduce, and pass on those genes (Cosmides & Tooby, 2013). Complex behaviors such as aggression, violence, cooperation, communication, over-eating, and anxiety can be better understood through genetics, adaptation, and evolution.

Positive Psychology

, like humanistic psychology, focuses on understanding the ways that bring us to our full potential while emphasizing what makes life joyful. While much of psychology and therapy is about treating psychological disorders, positive psychology attempts to create a way to scientifically investigate the good parts of being human and the happiness that life can bring (refer to “What Is Positive Psychology?”). Martin Seligman (1942–present) was the founder of positive psychology in the 1990s when he was the president of the American Psychological Association (APA). A positive psychologist might investigate how family, friends, physical exercise, meditation, and spiritual beliefs can increase subjective well-being.

What Is Positive Psychology?

This video discusses how positive psychology was developed, its focus and purpose, and how it differs from other approaches.

Neuroscience Approach

A or biological approach to psychology focuses on how the body and brain activity help explain complex behaviors and mental processes (refer to Figure 1.5). Someone taking this approach might look at how hormones affect emotions, how levels of certain brain chemicals inform us about depression or addiction, how cellular changes reflect learning, and how the activity within certain regions of the brain gives us a better understanding of visual perception. The approach overlaps with evolutionary psychology as they are both interested in the role that genetics plays in psychology. This is a relatively new approach to psychology but is growing rapidly with the tremendous advancements in neuroimaging technology, genomics, neurochemistry, and neurobiology.

Figure 1.5 Neuroscience

A neuroscience approach is interested in the biological and neurological basis of behavior and mental processes. Research in this area often uses neuroimaging techniques, as shown here.

Two researchers in a lab point to a screen with an image of the brain. In a chair a person sits with wires attached to their head.

Key Takeaways

  1. The early Greeks were the first to start seeing the brain as the location of thought and reason, and during the Renaissance artists began incorporating images of the brain into their artwork.
  2. We see scientific reasoning and methods emerge during the Age of Enlightenment.
  3. René Descartes proposed the idea of dualism, which is the separation of the mind (nonphysical) and the brain (physical), though most modern psychologists see them as one (monism).
  4. Psychology began to develop as a natural science with researchers such as Wilhelm Wundt, who thought we could understand the mind by understanding the underlying components in an approach called structuralism.
  5. Psychologists such as William James proposed functionalism as an approach to understanding the mind and behavior, which is focused on the function that thoughts and perceptions serve. 
  6. The psychoanalytic approach developed by Sigmund Freud argued that psychology could be understood by accessing unconscious or repressed thoughts and desires. 
  7. The approach of behaviorism focuses on observing and recording changes in behavior as evidence of learning governed by stimuli, rewards, and punishments.
  8. The cognitive revolution pushed back against the behaviorism approach and focused more on thoughts, perceptions, and consciousness. 
  9. Humanistic and positive psychology approaches focus on ways to maximize human potential. 
  10. A sociocultural approach to psychology looks at the interaction of society, culture, and ethnicity to better understand human behavior.
  11. The evolutionary and neuroscience approaches to psychology focus on the biological and genetic influences of thought and behavior.