1.3 Research Methods: Empiricism, Hypothesizing, and Experimental Design
Learning Objectives
Describe the five steps of the scientific method and why empiricism must be the first step in research.
Describe different ways of sampling from a population of participants and the advantages and disadvantages of each.
Define the WEIRD problem in psychological research.
Provide examples of ethical issues with using people in research and detail the efforts to protect and treat participants ethically.
Describe why non-human animals are used in psychological research and how they are protected against unethical treatment.
Explain the difference between an independent and a dependent variable and between a within-subject and a between-subject experimental design.
Explain the placebo effect.
Describe a correlation experiment and draw graphs showing positive and negative correlations.
Compare and contrast longitudinal and cross-sectional research designs.
Describe how to run a naturalistic observation experiment.
Explain why a case study is a unique experimental design.
Describe how archival data research and a meta-analysis are similar yet different.
In the final two sections of this chapter, we discuss the many ways of conducting research in psychology. Trying to understand the incredible complexity of human and animal behavior is not an easy task. However, to progress in this field, researchers over the last 150 years have tried to “treat psychology as a natural science” (refer to the William James quote in Chapter 1 “The Science of Psychology”). This requires following the scientific method, striving for objectivity in collecting data, and relying on the results of sophisticated statistical analysis when developing theories. This section discusses the five steps of the scientific method, notes how to choose and ethically treat human and animal participants in research, and reviews several types of experimental designs.
The scientific method A reliable way of investigating the natural world that has five steps: empiricism, hypothesis, experimentation, theorizing, and replication. is a reliable way to uncover information about how the world works. The scientific method is not some mystical, sophisticated process unique to professors and white-coated researchers; it is a way that humans are naturally inclined to understand the world. We observe the world around us, learn from the past, make predictions, test our ideas, and use evidence and reason to help explain events in hopes of making better sense of the world in the future. Advances in biomedical research, neuroscience, and psychology have benefited from following the scientific method of inquiry. Typically, the scientific method consists of five steps:
Empiricism (observation): Start with observations and assumptions about the natural or physical world. For example, we observe that people who live closer to the north or south poles have a higher rate of depression, especially during the winter months.
Hypothesize: Create a testable prediction of how we think something works. For example, reduced exposure to sunlight during the winter increases symptoms of depression.
Experimentation and Data Analysis: The first step in running an experiment is to choose participants. The next step is to design a well-controlled experiment created specifically to test the hypothesis. We collect data carefully to be as objective as possible. We then use statistics to make conclusions about whether the data supports or fails to support our hypothesis.
Theorize: After our data analysis, we put the experiment results into the context of other research findings to create theories. A theory is well supported by the results of experimentation. A theory helps explain what we observe but may uncover what is still unclear, creating new research ideas. Theories are modified as new information is discovered.
Replication: Repeat the experiment to make sure the results are consistent and reliable.
The stages of the scientific method cover a great deal of information, so let’s break it up. We’ll look at empiricism, hypothesizing, and experimental design in this section and then discuss data analysis, theorizing, and replication in Chapter 1, Section 4 “Scientific Research Methods: Data Analysis, Theorizing, and Replication”.
Empiricism
Empiricism is the idea that science begins with the observation of the natural world through our senses. That is, science cannot investigate the nature of angels, ghosts, wizards, or gods but must keep to what we can sense in the physical world. This idea can get a bit tricky with psychology because it is difficult to observe the workings of the mind, especially related to perceptions and consciousness. This is why behaviorists like B. F. Skinner struggled with theories about learning that relied on an unobservable internal or mental process (Skinner, 1984). Observation can take many forms. One might sit and watch children on a playground or have conversations with students in a college course, but most observations come from reading and learning about past research.
Let’s think about doing some research and follow the process through the scientific method. Suppose we are interested in how sleep quality affects learning and success in college. We might observe that students drifting off during lectures and complaining of poor sleep also struggle with exams and assignments. However, we will also read about sleep, memory, attention, and motivation.
Hypothesize
The next stage in the scientific method is to form a testable hypothesis The testable statement of prediction of the causal factors of what has been observed. that is created from observations. A hypothesis makes a prediction of the causal factors of what has been observed. A hypothesis is not a question nor a whim but comes from studying past research or already having a good understanding of the field you are studying.
So, let’s look at a couple of possible hypotheses that we can develop for our sleep and academic performance study.
Hypothesis 1. Students who are asked to remember facts about a college course topic will better recall the facts if they sleep between learning the facts and recalling them.
Hypothesis 2. Students who report better quality and consistency of sleep will have a higher college GPA.
Now that we have created testable hypotheses, we need to seriously consider how we design our experiment, which includes selecting, recruiting, and ethically treating the pool of research participants in our study.
Experimental Methods
The third step in the scientific method is experimentation. Let’s design an experiment for our first hypothesis about sleep and memory recall, beginning with how to select research participants.
Research Participants
For our sleep study, we’ll need some participants. Suppose we are interested in how sleep affects college students’ memory and academic performance; the best way to run our experiment is by using every college student on the planet as our participant pool. That’s impractical and impossible, you say? I agree. College students are considered the population, or the group in which we are interested, so we must draw a sample of people who represent our population well. There are several ways of collecting the sample:
A self-selected sample A method for recruiting research participants where participants often volunteer to receive course credit or financial compensation. (also called a volunteer sample) is when researchers recruit people for participation in their study. Sometimes participants are paid with money or gift cards for participating in research, or they may receive points in a psychology course.
A random sample A method for recruiting participants for an experiment, where the researcher makes a choice on how to randomly select the sample of the population, such as a randomly generated list of emails, phone numbers, or addresses. is when every person in the target population has an equal chance of participating in the research by using email addresses or phone numbers. Random samples are problematic in that not everyone in the target population is accessible, and people still must volunteer once asked. Random sampling research design can also be costly and time-consuming.
Psychology’s WEIRD Problem
Most research in psychology is conducted in universities by faculty members and students, so many participants come from introductory psychology courses or the main student body. Undergraduate college students taking an introduction to psychology course are not a good representation of the human population. Not only is a large portion of psychological research conducted on undergraduate college students, but participants are also typically overwhelmingly from Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic societies, or what has come to be called the WEIRD problem A problem in psychology where research participants are not a good representation of the whole human population, as the participants are often from western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic societies. in psychological research (Prayogshala, 2020). These problems create potential biases in the research and a tendency to overgeneralize the results to people from different cultures, socioeconomic statuses, education levels, and experiences.
Arnett (2009) evaluated articles from different subdisciplines in American Psychological Association (APA) journals and found 70 percent of participants were from the United States, and 96 percent were from WEIRD countries (refer to “The Hidden Biases in WEIRD Psychology Research”). Recently, Arnett and colleagues revaluated APA journals and found a slight improvement, with about 60 percent (a drop of 10 percent) of participants coming from the United States and 89 percent coming from WEIRD countries (Thalmayer, Toscanelli, & Arnett, 2020). Understanding these deficits is difficult because this demographic information has not been traditionally assessed or reported in psychological journals (Rad, Martingano, & Gingers, 2018).
The Hidden Biases in WEIRD Psychology Research
This video talks about biases in sampling problems in psychological research.
There are concerted efforts to broaden the scope and diversity of participants recruited for psychological studies and to increase the diversity of those conducting the research. Many journals require authors to publish demographic information about their participants even when this information is not a specific variable in their design. Rad et al. (2018) made several suggestions for authors and publishers to reduce the WEIRD problem in psychological research:
Explicitly tie the results to the specific population that was represented by the sample.
Write a justification for how and why the sample was collected.
Discuss the degree to which the findings could be generalized to a broader population, as well as the limitations.
Ethical Treatment of Participants
Using human subjects in experiments has a troubled history of exploitation, racism, and cruelty. In the early part of the twentieth century, no rules governed the use of human subjects in medical or psychological research. In the 1940s, the Nazis conducted horrific and torturous medical experiments on those held in concentration camps. This led to the Nuremberg Code, established in 1948, which is an international document requiring voluntary and informed consent by human participation in research (Komesaroff et al., 2002). From 1932 to 1972, the Public Health Service in the United States conducted a study on the effects of syphilis on Black men in the infamous Tuskegee Syphilis Study, in which the men were misinformed about the purpose of the study and were not given proper treatment for syphilis even when a treatment (penicillin) became available in the 1950s. The Tuskegee Syphilis Study led to a National Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral Research (NCPHS). In 1979, the NCPHS created the Belmont Report (Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, 1979) that laid out three basic principles for conducting ethical research with human participants:
The Belmont Report informed the ethical treatment of human participants in psychological research. Guidelines and principles for working with human subjects are typically very similar across universities. When planning a research project involving humans, a proposal is written containing detailed information, a literature review and the purpose of the study, and a description of the methods and the informed consent form, so participants know the parameters of the experiments and their rights. The proposal also provides the committee with details about the intended participants and how they might benefit from being involved in the experiment (e.g., money, course credit, education) as well as possible risks to their physical or psychological well-being. An internal review board (IRB) A group of colleagues and representatives from the community that evaluates whether the proposed research will treat participants ethically. evaluates the proposal and determines if the experiment can proceed, if more information is needed in the proposal, or if it is rejected. There has been psychological research carried out in the past that would likely not pass an IRB evaluation today (refer to “Five Psychology Experiments You Couldn't Do Today”).
Five Psychology Experiments You Couldn't Do Today
This video provides examples of famous studies in psychology that would be considered unethical today.
Animals in Research
Animals are used in several lines of research in psychology. The foundation for using animals in psychological research is the strong evidence that there is continuity in the fundamental processes underlying behavior in many animals, including humans.
Figure 1.12 Pigeon in Research
A pigeon in a research apparatus where the animal sees stimuli on a screen and food pellets are dispensed (on the left) when it does specific behaviors.

Source: © 2015 Levenson et al., https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0141357. Available under CC BY 4.0, https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
Today, rats and pigeons make up approximately 90 percent of animals used in psychological research (APA, 2010). Pigeons are used in research on learning and cognition because they see color and are highly motivated to receive food rewards (refer to Figure 1.12). Rats and mice are often used in research because they live for two years (which allows for aging studies), are relatively inexpensive to keep and maintain, are easy to keep in a limited space, are easily motivated by food, and learn quickly. Another recent advantage of using rodents in research is the development of techniques to alter genetics through inbreeding, crossbreeding, or changing or removing selected genes through genetic engineering.
Strict guidelines and oversight protect animals that are used in biomedical and psychological research. The Animal Welfare Act, created in 1996, established rules and regulations for the ethical treatment of animals. Animal use is overseen by the United States Department of Agriculture (USDA) and the Animal and Plant Health Inspection Service (APHIS). These organizations set strict guidelines for how animals are housed, fed, used in research, and euthanized. There are also strict guidelines about how and why research is performed with animals.
Methods of Experimentation
In an experiment, an independent variable In an experiment, it is what is manipulated by the researcher. is a factor that is changed or is manipulated by the researcher, and the dependent variable The measurement or data collected in an experiment that has the potential to reflect changes in the independent variable. is the measurement or data we collect. Researchers often look at the causal relationship between how changes to the independent variable affect the dependent variable.
In creating our experiment, we form a list of facts that one might come across in college about biology, geography, art, and anthropology. We divide the college student participants in our research into two groups. Group 1 learns the list of facts at 10:00 p.m. and tests themselves at 10:00 a.m. after a night’s sleep. The other group learns the list at 10:00 a.m. and is tested for what they remember at 10:00 p.m. after a day of no sleep, not even a nap in the afternoon. The independent variable in our experiment is sleep, which has two levels, sleep or no sleep. The dependent variable would be the number of facts they remember when tested twelve hours later. This is known as a between-subject design An experimental protocol where different groups experience the levels of the independent variable. because a participant in the experiment only experiences one level of the independent variable.
We could have also conducted this experiment as a within-subject design An experimental protocol where the same individuals experience different levels of the independent variable. where all participants experience both levels of the independent variable. We would run the experiment as a within-subject design by having participants on one day learn a set of facts in the morning and test at night, and a few days later, the same participants would learn a different set of facts at night and test the next morning. We would need to balance the order of experiencing the levels of the independent variables. Half of the participants would do the non-sleep condition before the sleep condition, and the order would be reversed for the other half.
Designing and conducting a good experiment can be challenging because there are often unintended extraneous or confounding variables An extraneous variable that unintentionally influences the results. that can influence results. Sherlock Holmes, Sir Arthur Conan Doyle's famous literary detective, said, “Once you eliminate the impossible, whatever remains, no matter how improbable, must be the truth” (Doyle, 2011). A researcher does their best to eliminate the impossible. Suppose in our sleep experiment, it was found that facts were remembered with greater accuracy after a night’s sleep. We might conclude that sleep facilitates memory, but can you think of a possible confounding variable? Perhaps it was not memory but the time of day that caused the difference. Maybe memory recall is just better in the morning regardless of whether one slept or not, or perhaps memory formation is better at night.
In psychology research, we often look for changes in complex behaviors, such as perception, learning, emotions, moods, or a decrease in symptoms of a disease or disorder. This can lead to a problem of subjective (personal) interpretation of changes in behavior by the researcher and the human participant. For example, suppose someone is conducting research on the effectiveness of an antidepressant. One might conduct an experiment where half of the participants get the antidepressant, and the other group gets a placebo, which is a pill that looks the same but does not have the active ingredient of the antidepressant. A placebo control group is necessary because there are often changes brought on by just the act of taking a pill. This is called the placebo effect The behavior changes that are observed when someone thinks they have taken medication or performed a behavior that they believe will reduce symptoms of a physical or psychological disorder. , and taking a placebo pill has been shown to reduce pain as well as reduce symptoms of depression and anxiety. If a person believes they are taking a pill to reduce pain, they express feeling less pain (refer to “The Power of the Placebo Effect—Emma Bryce”). The placebo effect tends to be short-lived but can still affect the results of an experiment.
The Power of the Placebo Effect—Emma Bryce
This animated video by TED-Ed discusses the history of the placebo effect.
A placebo or other type of control group allows all participants to have the same experience in the research except for the specific level of an independent variable. In our antidepressant study, all groups meet with a researcher, are assigned pills to take, monitor their behavior, and report back to the experimenter. The only difference is what is in the pill, which the participant does not know—this is called a single-blind A design where the participant does not know the experimental design or the level of the independent variable they will receive. experimental design because the participant is “blind” to what is in the pill. However, suppose the researcher knew that a specific participant received the antidepressant, not the placebo. In that case, the researcher might look for signs that the participant is in a better mood and disregard other, more negative behaviors or emotions. To help reduce the problems of subjective biases, researchers and participants in the experiment are often unaware of who is getting what level of an independent variable until the research is completed. This is known as a double-blind An experimental protocol where neither the research participant nor the researcher has knowledge of the experimental condition experienced by the participant. This adds objectivity to the experiment. experimental design (refer to “Explaining Randomization in Clinical Trials”).
Explaining Randomization in Clinical Trials
This animated video created by the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services discusses why double-blind and randomization techniques are used in research in several fields.
One helpful tool to reduce subjectivity is to quantify the results (as numbers) and run a statistical analysis on the data to conclude whether the results supported or failed to support the hypothesis. Regardless of whether a hypothesis is supported or not, the results provide information that can be put into the development of new hypotheses and new experimental designs. Let’s examine some different experimental designs used in psychology.
Correlational Research Design
A correlation is when two or more measurements change together in predictable ways. In a correlational research design A research design where the experimenter looks for how data collected about different variables change or do not change together. , the researcher does not have to manipulate the experience of the participants by changing levels of an independent variable (though they can); they simply collect data and see how different variables change or don’t change together. Two variables can increase or decrease together, which is called a positive correlation, or as one variable increases, the other can decrease, which is a negative correlation.
Let’s take the example of our second hypothesis in our sleep study. Suppose we give a survey to one hundred students and ask several questions about their sleep habits as well as their school performance, and we find that the average number of hours of sleep per night shows a positive correlation with their cumulative GPA—as sleep hours increase, GPA increases. Scatter plots A graphing design where a single point represents an individual’s response on two variables presented on the x- and y-axis of the graph—often used to demonstrate correlations. are typically used to showcase correlational data, with the horizontal x-axis indicating one variable (e.g., average hours slept per night) and the vertical y-axis indicating the other variable (e.g., GPA) (refer to A in Figure 1.13). We could also look at another aspect of sleep, such as the number of times a person wakes up during the night compared with GPA, and find a negative correlation (refer to B in Figure 1.13). Finally, we see no correlation between the time someone gets up in the morning and GPA (refer to C in Figure 1.13).
Figure 1.13 Scatter Plots and Correlations
These scatter plots show a positive correlation (A), negative correlation (B), and no correlation (C) between cumulative GPA and aspects of sleep. The black dotted line is the line that best fits all the data.

Source: Martin Shapiro
Long Description
Three graphs are stacked. Each one shows 0 to 4.5 on the y-axis labeled “Cumulative GPA”. Graph A is labeled “Positive correlation” and the x-axis goes from 0 to 12 labeled “Average Number of Hours of Sleep Per Night”. A dashed line starts at (5, 2.4) and extends upward to (9, 3.6). The scattered dots are clustered above and below the line and nowhere else on the graph. Graph B is labeled “Negative correlation” with the x-axis from 0 to 8 labeled “Average Number of Times Waking Up During the Night”. A dashed line starts at (0, 3.7) and slopes downward to (7, 2) and dots are scattered above and below the line. Graph C is labeled “No correlation” and with the x-axis labeled “Average Wake Time” going from 5:02 to 12:14. A horizontal line goes from 5:36 to 11:36. Dots are scattered above and below the line.
We need to be careful not to fall into the problem of correlation automatically implying causation. Looking at A in Figure 1.13, one might assume that increased sleep caused GPA to increase. While sleep might be a contributing factor in getting better grades, there may also be other factors that cause the increase in both. Can you think of a factor that might affect both sleep and grades? What about the level of stress a person is experiencing? Someone experiencing a stressful life may have trouble sleeping and have difficulty concentrating in class and studying. One of the goals of correlation research is to try to understand better why variables change together.
Longitudinal Research Design
A longitudinal research design An experimental design that involves recording the same measurements over a short or long period of time, often from the same participants. involves recording the same measurements over a short or long period of time. Developmental psychologists often use longitudinal research to track how age, time, and experiences change measurements of memory, life satisfaction, drug use, reasoning about morality, sleep patterns, sexual behavior, and many other psychological factors. For example, the Minnesota Twin Study has been surveying identical and fraternal twins for decades to better understand the genetic and environmental influence on psychological measures (Segal, 2012). Longitudinal data are often presented as line graphs with time along the horizontal x-axis and the dependent variable on the vertical y-axis. For example, suppose a researcher is interested in how someone would score on a sensation-seeking survey as they age. The survey might ask questions about how much someone might enjoy roller-coaster riding, sky diving, and cliff jumping if they were given the opportunity (refer to Figure 1.14).
Figure 1.14 Longitudinal Research Design
The same people are given a survey about how they value sensation-seeking activities every five years from age 10 to age 45. Their average score is shown on the y-axis, and their age is on the x-axis. One can see that the values of sensation-seeking activities peak in adolescence and decrease with advancing age.

Source: Martin Shapiro
Long Description
The y-axis is labeled “Average sensation-seeking value score” with values from 0.0 to 5.0. The x-axis is labeled “Age when assessed” from 0 to 45. The line begins at (5, 4.1) goes up to (15, 4.5) and then continues on points downward ending at (45, 1.8).
Cross-Sectional Research Design
A longitudinal research design is a powerful way of tracking changes at different ages, but it can be expensive, and because it may take several years, researchers run the risk of losing contact with participants along the way. Another way to investigate age-related differences is to conduct a cross-sectional design An experimental design that samples from a diverse population, such as different age groups. in which a sample of the population at different age groups is evaluated and compared. For example, suppose a researcher is interested in how the prevalence of drug addiction changes from adolescence to middle adulthood. They might provide a survey that evaluates problematic drug use to fifty people in different age groups. A histogram or bar graph might be used to display the results (refer to Figure 1.15).
Figure 1.15 Cross-Sectional Design
A sample of participants was taken from different age groups and asked their agreement or disagreement with the following statement: “I find it difficult to do what I should at school, work, or home because of drug use.” The different age groups are noted along the x-axis, and the percentage that agreed with this statement is on the y-axis.

Source: Martin Shapiro
Naturalistic Observation
A naturalistic observation A research method that takes a hands-off approach to data collection, such as watching children interact in a classroom. method of research takes a hands-off approach to data collection. The researcher puts themselves in a position where they observe the group of people, watch videos, and take notes, but do not interfere or interact with subjects of the research. This could happen in a laboratory by watching people through a one-way mirror, but it is often carried out in the field by observing children in a classroom, shoppers at a grocery store, or employees working with equipment in a factory, for instance. Naturalistic observation techniques are used in several other fields, such as anthropology and zoology. Jane Goodall moved to the Gombe Reserve in Africa in 1957 and observed chimpanzees in the wild for several years, where she discovered many unique things about these apes, including that they used tools.
Naturalistic observation lacks the ability to control participants’ experiences and can run the risk of subjective interpretation of what is observed, but it has the advantage of conducting research in more realistic and complex environments. One way to help reduce subjectivity is for researchers to watch videos and score what they see using an agreed upon rubric describing specific behaviors.
Case Study
Research in psychology often involves evaluating many participants, which allows for greater generalization of the findings to other members of the population. However, there are times we can learn a great deal from single individuals with unique problems, circumstances, or abilities. An in-depth investigation of one person or situation is known as a case study A method where research is conducted with one participant who has unique characteristics. . A famous case study we will learn more about in Chapter 7 “Motivation and Emotions” is that of Henry Molaison (known as H. M.), who had a large section of his brain removed when he was a child to prevent severe epileptic seizures. After his surgery, H. M. was not able to make new long-term memories. As one would imagine, a researcher could not replicate these situations in the controlled setting of a laboratory, but case studies can provide insights into neuroscience, memory, and psychological disorders by looking at real-world examples that cannot be produced in a lab.
Archival Data Research
Several large research projects collect a great deal of data that they make available to other researchers and, in some cases, the general public. This archival data A method of conducting research by evaluating data that has already been collected and made available to the researcher. set often has demographic information about sex, age, ethnicity, income, political affiliation, religion, and location. It might also include results of survey questions on topics related to psychology, such as stress levels, childhood experiences, or life satisfaction. Research with specific questions can then use the archival data instead of collecting their own to test hypotheses. This APA webpage has links to several archival data sets on topics ranging from child language to adolescent health.
Meta-Analysis
Like archival data research, a meta-analysisA method of research where results from many studies that investigate the same or similar topic are reviewed and summarized. It is good for looking at larger trends. does not develop and run new experiments but instead carefully evaluates previously acquired data. In a meta-analysis, researchers use statistics to evaluate the results of multiple scientific studies investigating the same question. This approach has the advantage of not relying on the results of a single study for answers, as it looks at larger trends by evaluating the results of many studies. Scientists often write systematic reviews of a specific topic where they synthesize the findings of many decades of research, and they often use a meta-analysis in forming conclusions. A colleague of mine, Dr. Chris Miller, and his students conduct meta-analyses of fMRI brain imaging studies that are interested in psychological disorders. For example, in a recent line of research, Dr. Miller and his team searched for all imaging studies that have looked at depression, and then they created computer models that evaluate patterns of brain activity across the images from all the studies. In later sections, we see how meta-analysis works well to combat several of the problems in psychological research (for review, refer to Stone & Rosopa, 2017).
Key Takeaways
The five steps of the scientific method are empiricism, hypothesizing, experimentation and data analysis, theorizing, and replication.
Empiricism is the philosophy that all scientific inquiry begins with observations of the natural or physical world.
A hypothesis is developed from observation and is a testable prediction.
There are several ways to sample a population, including convenience, self-select, and random sample.
Often participants in psychological research are overrepresented by participants who are from western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic societies, known as the WEIRD problem.
A university’s internal review board (IRB) evaluates research proposals to make sure participants are treated ethically.
Animals are used in psychological research because there is an assumption of common principles and mechanisms underlying many aspects of humans.
Experimental variables:
Independent variables are manipulated by the researcher.
Dependent variable is what is measured.
Confounding variables unintentionally influence results.
A between-subjects design is when different groups experience different levels of an independent variable. A within-subjects experiment is when the same group experiences different levels of the independent variable.
A placebo is a pill without an active ingredient and is used to control for any changes one might see from just being part of the study and taking a pill.
Single-blind and double-blind experimental designs help to reduce subjective interpretations of the effects of the independent variables.
Experimental designs and methods:
Correlation: looks at how two measurements change together.
Longitudinal: tracks change to the same measurement over time.
Cross-sectional: conducts the same measurements with people of different ages at the same time.
Naturalistic observation: collects data without interfering with the participants.
Case studies: investigation of unique individuals.
Archival data: analyzes different components of previously collected data.
Meta-analysis: uses statistics to look for overarching trends in many studies.